Publications (2007-2001)

Books (Edited)
 
2006: The handbook on functional neuroimaging of cognition (2nd Edition). MIT Press.
2001: Handbook on functional neuroimaging of cognition. MIT Press.

Chapters

  • Giesbrecht, B., Kingstone, A., Handy, T, & Mangun, G. R. (2006). Functional neuroimaging of human attention. In R. Cabeza and A. Kingstone (Eds.) Handbook on functional neuroimaging of cognition (2nd Edition). (pp. 85-111). MIT Press (2nd Edition).
  • Kingstone, A., Smilek, D., Birmingham, E., Cameraon, D. & Bischof, W.F. (2005). Cognitive ethology: Giving real life to attention research. In J. Duncan, L. Phillips & P. McLeod (Eds.) Measuring the mind: Speed, control & age. (pp. 341-358). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Soto-Faraco, S. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Multisensory integration of dynamic information. In G. Calvert, C. Spence & B. Stein (Eds.). Handbook of Multisensory Processes. (pp. 49-68) . MIT Press.

Papers

  • Birmingham, E., Visser, T., Snyder, J. & Kingstone, A. (2007). Inhibition of return: Unraveling a paradox. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 957-63.
  • Birmingham, E., Bischof, W. F. & Kingstone, A. (2007). Why do we look at eyes? Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1 (1), 1-6.
  • Chapman, C., Hunt, A. & Kingstone, A. (2007). Squeezing uncertainty from saccadic compression. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1 (2), 1-4.
  • Dalrymple, K. A., Kingstone, A. & Barton, J.J.S. (2007). Seeing trees or seeing forests in simultanagnosia: Attentional capture can be local or global., Neuropsychologia, 45, 871-875.
  • Hunt, A., Cooper, R. M., Hungr, C. & Kingstone, A. (2007). The effect of emotional faces on eye movements and attention. Visual Cognition, 15, 513-531.
  • Hunt, A., von Mühlenen, A. & Kingstone, A. (2007). The timecourse of attentional and oculomotor capture reveals a common cause. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 271-284.
  • Morein-Zamir, S., Chua, R., Franks, I. Negelkerke, P. & Kingstone, A. (2007). Predictability influences stopping and response control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 149-162.
  • Smilek, D., Eastwood, J. D. Reynolds, M. G. & Kingstone, A. (2007). Metacognitive errors in change detection: Missing the gap between lab and life. Consciousness & Cognition, 16, 52-57.
  • Ristic, J. Wright, A. Kingstone, A. (2007). Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 964-969.
  • Snyder, J. & Kingstone, A. (2007). Inhibition of return at multiple locations and its impact on visual search. Visual Cognition, 15, 238-256.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A. & Spence, C. (2007). Integrating motion information across sensory modalities: The role of top-down factors. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 54-67.
  • van Zoest, W., Lleras, A., Kingstone, A. F., & Enns, J. T. (2007). In sight, out of mind: The role of eye movements in the rapid resumption of visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1204-1217.
  • Morein-Zamir, S. & Kingstone, A. (2006). Fixation offset and stop signal intensity effects on saccadic countermanding: A crossmodal investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 453-462.
  • Morein-Zamir, S., Negelkerke, P., Chua, R., Franks, I. & Kingstone, A. (2006). Measuring online volitional response control with a continuous tracking task Behavior Research Methods, 38, 638-647.
  • Gibson, B.S. & Kingstone, A. (2006). Visual attention and the semantics of space: Beyond central and peripheral cues. Psychological Science, 17, 626-627.
  • van Zoest, W., Giesbrecht, B., Enns, J. T., & Kingstone, A. (2006). New reflections on visual search: Inter-item symmetry matters! Psychological Science, 17, 535-542.
  • Morein-Zamir, S., Negelkerke, P., Chua, R., Franks, I. & Kingstone, A. (2006). Compatibility effects in stopping and response initiation in a continuous tracking task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 2148-2161.
  • Olk, B., Change, E., Kingstone, A. & Ro, T. (2006). Modulation of antisaccades by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 76-82.
  • Ristic, J., Wright, A. & Kingstone, A. (2006). The number line effect reflects top-down control. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 862-868.
  • Ristic, J. & Kingstone, A. (2006). Attention to arrows: Pointing to a new direction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1921-1930.
  • Shimozaki, S., Kingstone, A., Olk, B., Stowe, B. & Eckstein, M (2006). Classification images of two right hemisphere patients: A window into the attentional mechanisms of spatial neglect. Brain Research, 1080, 26-52.
  • Smilek, D., Birmingham, E., Cameron, D., Bischof, W.F. & Kingstone, A. (2006). Cognitive ethology and exploring attention in real world scenes. Brain Research, 1080, 101-119.
  • Borowsky, R., Loehr, J., Friesen, C.K., Kraushaar, G., Kingstone A., & Sarty, G. (2005). Modularity and intersection of ‘what’, ‘where’, and ‘how’ processing of visual stimuli: A New Method of fMRI Localization. Brain Topography, 18, 67-75.
  • Friesen, C. K., Moore, C. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Does gaze direction really trigger a shift of spatial attention? Brain & Cognition. 57, 66-69.
  • Medler, D.A., Dawson, M.R.W. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Functional localization and double dissociations: the relationship between internal structure and behavior. Brain & Cognition, 57, 146-150.
  • Miller, M. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Taking the high road on subcortical transfer. Brain and Cognition, 57, 162-164.
  • Ristic, J. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Taking control of reflexive social attention. Cognition, 94, B55-65.
  • Ristic, J., Mottron, L., Friesen, C. K., Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Eyes are special but not for everyone: The case of autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 715-718.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Morein-Zamir, S. & Kingstone, A. (2005). On audiovisual spatial synergy: The fragility `of the phenomenon. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 444-457.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Sinnett, S. Alsius, A. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Spatial orienting of tactile attention induced by gaze direction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1024-1031.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Spence, C. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Assessing the automaticity in the visual capture of auditory apparent motion. Acta Psychologica, 118, 47-70.
  • Tipper, C. & Kingstone, A. (2005). Is inhibition of return a reflexive effect? Cognition, 97, B55-62.
  • Austen, E., Soto-Faraco, S., Enns, J. T. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Mislocations of touch to a fake hand. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 170-181.
  • Chaston, A. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Time estimation: The effect of cortically mediated attention. Brain & Cognition, 55, 286-289.
  • Donovan, C-L, Lindsay, S. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Flexible and abstract resolutions to crossmodal conflicts. Brain & Cognition, 56, 118-122.
  • Fecteau, J. H., Kingstone, A. & Enns, J. T. (2004). Hemisphere differences in explicit and implicit word reading. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 550-554.
  • Friesen, C. K., Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 319-329.
  • Giesbrecht, B. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Right hemisphere involvement in the attentional blink: Evidence from a split-brain patient. Brain & Cognition, 55, 303-306.
  • Giesbrecht, B., Bischof, W. F. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Seeing the light: Adapting luminance reveals low-level visual processes in the attentional blink. Brain & Cognition, 55, 307-309.
  • Grabowecky, M. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Can semantic information be transferred between hemispheres in the split-brain? Brain & Cognition, 55, 310-313.
  • Hunt, A. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Multisensory executive functioning. Brain & Cognition, 55, 325-327.
  • Hunt, A., Olk, B., von Mühlenen, A. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Integration of competing saccade programs. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 206-208.
  • Kingstone, A., Tipper, C., Ristic, J., & Ngan, E. (2004). The eyes have it!: An fMRI investigation. Brain & Cognition, 55, 269-271.
  • Morein-Zamir, S., Negelkerke, P., Chua, R., Franks, I. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Stopping a prepared response versus ongoing response: Is there more than one kind of stopping? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1034-1140.
  • Olk, B., Wee, J. & Kingstone, A. (2004). The effect of hemispatial neglect on the perception of centre. Brain & Cognition, 55, 365-367.
  • Roggeveen, A., Kingstone, A. & Enns, J. (2004). Influence of inter-item symmetry in visual search. Spatial Vision, 17, 443-464.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Spence, C., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Crossmodal dynamic capture. congruency effects in the perception of motion across sensory modalities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 330-345.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Spence, C., Lloyd. D., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Moving multisensory research along: Motion perception across sensory modalities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 29–32.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Spence, C. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Congruency effects between auditory and tactile motion: Extending the cross-modal dynamic capture phenomenon. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 208-217.
  • Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Attentional capture modulates perceptual sensitivity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 551-554.
  • Valsangkar-Smyth, M. A., Donovan, C-L., Sinnett, S., Dawson, M.R.W. & Kingstone, A. (2004). Hemispheric performance in object-based attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 84-91.
  • Wig, G. S., Miller, M. Kingstone, A., & Kelley, W. (2004). Separable routes to human memory formation: Dissociating task and material contributions in prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 139–148.
  • Friesen, C. K. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Covert and overt orienting to gaze direction cues and the effects of fixation offset. Neuroreport, 14, 489-93.
  • Friesen, C. K. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Abrupt onsets and gaze cues trigger independent reflexive attention effects. Cognition, 87, B1-B10.
  • Giesbrecht, B., Bischof, W. F. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Visual masking during the attentional blink: Tests of the object substitution hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 238-258.
  • Hunt, A. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Inhibition of return: Dissociating attentional and oculomotor components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 348-354.
  • Hunt, A. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Covert and overt voluntary attention: Linked or independent? Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 102 – 105.
  • Kingstone, A., Smilek, D., Ristic, J., Friesen, C. K. & Eastwood, J. D. (2003). Attention, researchers! It’s time to pay attention to the real world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 176-180.
  • Morein-Zamir, S., Soto-Faraco, S. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Auditory capture of vision: Examing temporal ventriloquism. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 154-163.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A. & Spence, C. (2003). Multisensory contributions to motion perception Neuropsychologia, 41, 1847-1863.
  • Olk, B. & Kingstone, A. (2003). Why are antisaccades slower than prosaccades? A novel finding using a new paradigm. Neuroreport, 14, 151-155.
  • Caclin, A., Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A., & Spence, C. Tactile capture of audition. (2002). Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 616-630.
  • Ristic, J., Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (2002). Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 507-513.
  • Kingstone, A., Danziger, S., Langton, S. R. H. & Soto-Faraco, S. (2002). Attentional capture: Biological relevance, multisensory stimulation, and consciousness. Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 23, 1-30.
  • Kingstone, A. , Klein, R.M., Morein-Zamir, S., Hunt, A., Maxner, C., & Fisk, J. (2002). Components of attention: Chronometric techniques and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 951-967.
  • Miller, M. Kingstone, A. & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2002). Hemispheric encoding asymmetry is more apparent than real. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 702-708.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Lyons, J., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Spence, C. & Kingstone, A. (2002). The ventriloquist in motion: Illusory capture of dynamic information across sensory modalities. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 139-146.
  • Soto-Faraco, S., Spence, C., Kingstone, A., Hillstrom, A. P. & Shapiro, K. (2002). A crossmodal attentional blink between vision and touch. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 731-738.
  • Danziger, S., Kingstone, A.& Ward, R. (2001). Environmentally defined frames of reference: Their sensitivity to spatial cues and attention, and their time course. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 494-503.
  • Fecteau, J. H., Enns, J. T., & Kingstone, A. (2001). Competition-induced visual field differences in search. Psychological Science, 11, 386-393.
  • Giesbrecht, B., Dixon, P., & Kingstone, A. (2001). Cued shifts of attention and memory encoding in partial report: A dual-task approach. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 695– 726.
  • Kingstone, A. (2001). Cognitive neuroscience and multidisciplinary research. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 81-82. 
  • Kingstone, A. (2001). Multisensory integration: A cognitive neuroscience approach. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 121-122.
  • Snyder, J. & Kingstone, A. (2001). Inhibition of return at multiple locations in visual search: When you see it and when you don’t. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 1221-1237.
  • Snyder, J., Schmidt, W. C. & Kingstone, A. (2001). Attentional momentum does not underlie inhibition of return. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1420-1432.
  • Spence, C., Kingstone, A., Shore, D. I., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2001). Representation of visuotactile space in the split-brain. Psychological Science, 12, 90-93.
  • Spence, C., Shore, D. I., Gazzaniga, M. S., Soto-Faraco, S., & Kingstone, A. (2001). Failure to remap visuotactile space across the midline in the split-brain. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 133-140.